Frost : Towards a universal model of reading 46
نویسندگان
چکیده
Letter-position tolerance varies across languages. This observation suggests that the neural code for letter strings may also be subtly different. Although language-specific models remain useful, we should endeavor to develop a universal model of reading acquisition which incorporates crucial neurobiological constraints. Such a model, through a progressive internalization of phonological and lexical regularities, could perhaps converge onto the language-specific properties outlined by Frost. “Cmabirdge” reads almost as well as “Cambridge,” but only in some languages. Ram Frost is right in pointing out that tolerance to letter-position swaps is not a universal feature of reading. His hypothesis that writing systems “optimally represent the languages’ phonological spaces” (sect. 3, para. 1) is appealing and is indeed a crucial consideration when discussing the possibility of spelling reform – some variations in writing systems may be more “rational” than they first appear (Dehaene 2009, pp. 32–37). Does it follow, however, that current open-bigrammodels of orthographic processing are, in Ram Frost’s words, “ill-advised”? And what is the best strategy to achieve a “universal model of reading”? From a neuroscientific perspective, much insight can be gained from limited models that consider in detail not only the problems raised by a specific script and language, but also the neurobiological constraints on how the brain might solve them. Our bigram neuron hypothesis, which postulates that the left occipitotemporal visual word form area (VWFA) may contain neurons tuned to ordered letter pairs, was presented in this context as a useful solution to position-invariant recognition of written words in English, French, and related Roman scripts (Dehaene et al. 2005). A functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) experiment aimed at testing the predictions of this model demonstrated that reading indeed relies on a hierarchy of brain areas sensitive to increasingly complex properties, from individual letters to bigrams and to higher-order combinations of abstract letter representations (Vinckier et al. 2007). These regions form a gradient of selectivity through the occipitotemporal cortex, with activation becoming more selective for higher-level stimuli towards the anterior fusiform region (Fig. 1) (see also Binder 2006). Interestingly, a similar gradient may also exist in Chinese script (Chan et al. 2009). It would be important to probe it in Hebrew readers. We agree with Frost that developing a more general, languageuniversal model of reading acquisition is a major goal for future research. However, crucially, we would add that such a universal model should incorporate strong constraints from brain architecture and not just linguistics. Existing connectionist models typically incorporate few neurobiological constraints and, as a result, provide information-processing solutions that need not be realistic at the brain level. Reading is a ventral visual stream process that “recycles” existing visual mechanisms used for object recognition (Dehaene 2009; Szwed et al. 2009; 2011; however, see Reich et al. 2011) As such, it is heavily constrained by the limitations of the visual brain, for example, the necessity to process information Figure 1 (Szwed et al.). Hierarchical Coding of Letter Strings in the Ventral Visual Stream. Up: Design and examples of stimuli used, with an increasing structural similarity to real words. Down: fMRl results The image illustrates the spatial layout of sensitivity of the occipitotemporal cortex to letter strings of different similarity to real words. Activations become more selective for higher-level stimuli (i.e., stimuli more similar to real words) toward the anterior fusiform regions. This is taken as evidence for a hierarchy of brain areas sensitive to increasingly complex properties, from individual letters to bigrams and to higher-order combinations of letters. (Adapted from Vinckier et al. 2007). Commentary/Frost: Towards a universal model of reading 46 BEHAVIORAL AND BRAIN SCIENCES (2012) 35:5 step by step through distinct visual areas with increasing receptive fields (V1, V2, V3, V4, V5, LO, MT...). Implementing these constraints into general models has proven very challenging so far (although see Mozer 1987). Indeed, important advances in the field have been predominantly guided by narrow, languagespecific theories that hardwire these constraints into their architectures. Nevertheless, the vast neurobiological knowledge about these regions should ultimately be tapped by a more general model. Starting from a generic, biologically realistic neuronal architecture, and using realistic synaptic plasticity rules, the future model would converge on a specific architecture for the VWFA in any language. It could include a Bayesian implementation of the informative fragments model, which falls close to predicting the real-life responses of ventral visual stream neurons involved in object recognition (Ullman 2007). Would such a model, once developed, substantiate Frost’s claim that the internal code for letter strings varies strongly across languages, depending on their phonology and word structure? Here, we should clear up a frequent confusion. During online processing, when an actual word is read by a fluent reader, magnetoencephalography (MEG) experiments, with their high temporal resolution, have shown that the first major response of the visual system, peaking roughly 130 msec after seeing a word, is determined overwhelmingly by the frequency of letter combinations that make up a word, whereas lexical and phonological effects come into play much later (Simos et al. 2002; Solomyak & Marantz 2010). Thus, in adults, the VWFA may reflect a relatively isolated stage of orthographic processing that is essentially immune to phonological and semantic influences (Dehaene & Cohen 2011; but see Price & Devlin 2011). However, this is not to say that, in the course of learning, the acquired orthographical code cannot be influenced by the needs of the phonological and semantic systems to which the VWFA ultimately projects. The anatomical localization of the VWFA is strongly influenced, not only by bottom visual constraints (Hasson et al. 2002), but also by the lateralization of the target spoken language (Pinel & Dehaene 2009). MEG shows that, in English readers, the visual word form system decomposes the words’ morphology into prefixes, roots, and affixes about 170 msec after stimulus onset (Solomyak & Marantz 2010). Such decomposition is automatic and operates even with pseudo-affixed words like “brother” that can be falsely decomposed into “broth” and “er” (Lewis et al. 2011). Thus, the visual system has internalized orthographic units that are relevant to morphological and lexical knowledge. Although not yet demonstrated, we consider it likely that the VWFA also codes for frequent substrings that facilitate the mapping onto phonemes, such as “th” or “ain” in English. Indeed, this hypothesis may explain why English reading, with its complex grapheme–phoneme mappings, causes greater activation in the VWFA than does Italian reading (Paulesu et al. 2000). In this context, we have no difficulty in accepting Frost’s argument that the optimal neural code for letter strings might have to be much less tolerant to letter swaps in Hebrew than in English. This view predicts root detectors in the more anterior part of VWFA of Hebrew readers and sharper tuning curves for letters and bigrams detectors. Testing such predictions for scripts other than Latin is an important goal for future neuroimaging experiments. A readily available tool is fMRI repetition suppression, which has proven sensitive to subtle properties of object, number, and letter tuning (Dehaene et al. 2004; Grill-Spector et al. 1999). Alternatively, multivariate pattern analysis may provide more direct access to the fine-tuning characteristic of the VWFA (Braet et al. 2012).
منابع مشابه
Towards a universal model of reading.
In the last decade, reading research has seen a paradigmatic shift. A new wave of computational models of orthographic processing that offer various forms of noisy position or context-sensitive coding have revolutionized the field of visual word recognition. The influx of such models stems mainly from consistent findings, coming mostly from European languages, regarding an apparent insensitivit...
متن کاملBehavioral and Brain Sciences Commentary Proposal on Frost: Giving theories of reading a sporting chance
The search for a universal theory of reading is misguided. Instead, theories should articulate general principles of neural computation that interact with language-specific learning environments to explain the full diversity of observed reading-related phenomena across the world's languages. Powered by Editorial Manager® and Preprint Manager® from Aries Systems Corporation Giving theories of re...
متن کاملPedagogy of Possibility for EFL Learners: Principles, Application, and Course Development
This study reports on the development of a reading comprehension course based on Critical Pedagogy principles, and the result of its application on EFL learners' reading comprehension competence, their motivation to read English materials, their democratic attitudes towards their English classrooms, and also their attitudes towards the critical course. The present study is, in fact, a step forw...
متن کاملAssessing the validity and reliability of the Persian version of the Isakson and et al. Reading Attitude Questionnaire
Purpose: This study Accomplished to evaluate the adequacy of psychometric properties and validate the Isaacson Reading Attitude Questionnaire. Isaacson et al. (2016) designed The Attitude Towards Academic Reading Questionnaire. Method: To validate this tool, apparent and content validity indices were measured. Also, confirmatory factor analysis was utilized to evaluate the validity of the stru...
متن کاملGender-based Study of Learners' Reading Ability Through Dynamic Assessment (DA): Guthke's Lerntest approach in focus
The intention of the present paper is to seek whether gender has any impact on the effectiveness of applying Guthke's Lerntest approach in reading comprehension. For this purpose, after selecting 60 intermediate EFL learners, they were arbitrarily categorized to four clusters of males and females, two experimental and two control groups, each consisting of 15 male and 15 female learners. Both c...
متن کامل