I Think, Therefore I Am (Unhappy)
نویسنده
چکیده
When not performing a task, we are usually engaged in a variety of thoughts, for instance about current problems, opportunities, our self-image, our relationships, about past experiences, and about plans for the future. Such mind wandering was found to occupy almost half of our time spent awake (Killingsworth and Gilbert, 2010). Mind wandering may have had the evolutionary advantage of better adaptation to changing environmental and social circumstances, because it allows the mental contemplation of issues remote from the here and now (Schooler et al., 2011). But mind wandering also has substantial downsides. Even when we are performing a task, our minds tend to wander. This divagation into situation-independent thoughts usually goes unnoticed and it leads to impaired task performance (e.g., Christoff et al., 2009; Schooler et al., 2011). Depending on the task, this actually may be dangerous, for instance when driving a vehicle (He et al., 2011). Furthermore, mind wandering is related to negative emotions (e.g., Smallwood et al., 2009). A recent study has demonstrated that people are less happy when their minds are wandering to neutral topics than when their minds are focused on their current activity (Killingsworth and Gilbert, 2010). And even when thinking about pleasant topics, they are not happier than when they focus on the task at hand (Killingsworth and Gilbert, 2010). Knowledge about the negative effects of mind wandering has been available for a long time in various religious traditions, in particular in Buddhism. The practice of mindfulness meditation aims at reducing the prevalence of uncontrolled thought chains by increasing meta-awareness of ongoing mental activities. The finding that mind wandering related brain activations (see below) are diminished in case of meta-awareness (Christoff et al., 2009) provides neuroscientific validation for this strategy. Today, elements of mindfulness meditation have been implemented in several therapeutic systems, such as mindfulnessbased stress reduction (Ludwig and KabatZinn, 2008) and mindfulness-based cognitive therapy (Segal et al., 2002). These techniques are applied, for instance, for the treatment of attention deficit disorder, psychosomatic disorders, and depression. But is there neuroscientific evidence that mindfulness training indeed reduces neural activations associated with mind wandering? Recent fMRI studies support this assumption. First of all, mind wandering was found to be associated with activations in so-called default mode brain areas (Mason et al., 2007; Christoff et al., 2009; Stawarczyk et al., 2011; Hasenkamp et al., 2012). These brain areas were demonstrated to exhibit increased activity during task-free periods and decreased activity during goaloriented behavior as measured by functional brain imaging (Raichle et al., 2001). The default mode network comprises, for instance, parts of medial prefrontal, medial temporal and parietal cortex, the posterior cingulate cortex, and the precuneus. A recent study investigated activations in default mode areas in subjects with no, moderate, and high meditation experience during daydreaming and mindfulness meditation (Ott et al., 2010). The authors observed an inhibition of default mode network activity during mindfulness meditation compared to the daydreaming condition. Moreover, the inhibition of anterior default mode areas, which have been argued to be associated with self-related and social cognition (Northoff et al., 2006), was more persistent in subjects with high compared to no meditation experience. In a similar vein, a recent investigation addressed the changes in brain activation during three types of mindfulness meditation in experienced meditators and meditation-naïve controls (Brewer et al., 2011). Compared to the controls experienced meditators reported less mind wandering during the meditation periods and the main nodes of the default mode network, medial prefrontal and posterior cingulate cortex, were deactivated across all meditation types. Moreover, functional connectivity analyses revealed in the experienced meditators versus controls increased coactivation of the posterior cingulate and the dorsal anterior cingulate (during resting state baseline and meditation), as well as the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex (during baseline), i.e., regions probably involved in self-monitoring and cognitive control (Brewer et al., 2011). This result suggests that processes supported by the default mode network are more accessible to monitoring and control in experienced meditators. The authors discuss that control regions may coactivate to monitor and dampen processes related to the default mode network when interfering with a task. They speculate that this coactivation may, over time, become a new “default mode” that is established not only during meditation, but also during the resting state. Another study addressed the neural correlates of conceptual processing in regular Zen (a particular kind of mindfulness meditation) practitioners compared to matched control subjects. Participants were required to decide whether sparsely presented visual stimuli were real English words or strings of letters with plausible readings but no semantic content (Pagnoni et al., 2008). The words, compared to the non-words, are likely to trigger a cascade of semantic associations. Moreover, participants were instructed to attend to their breathing throughout the scan and to promptly re-focus attention to their breathing after responding to the stimuli. The authors found that Zen practitioners, compared to controls, displayed a reduced duration of semantic processing effects in default mode brain areas suggesting greater ability to disengage from mind wandering. Furthermore, one investigation evaluated functional activations during two tasks associated with reading trait related adjectives (Farb et al., 2007). In one task, subjects should reflect on what the adjective meant about them as a person (narrative focus), while in the other task they should monitor their moment-to-moment experience in response to the adjectives (experiential focus). The authors recruited participants of an 8-week course in mindfulness meditation and novices. During the experiential compared to the narrative condition reductions
منابع مشابه
Animal Suffering: A Moorean Response to a Problem of Evil
In this paper, I propose a new response to a particular instance of the problem of evil: the problem of animal suffering. My solution, in brief, is that the rational theist may argue, justifiably, that an omnipotent, omniscient, omnibenevolent God would not allow gratuitous animal suffering to occur, and, therefore, all instances of animal suffering that are encountered are not instanc...
متن کاملEating poorly, no exercise, and feeling bad about it!
I feel I am getting fat, am eating poorly, and not getting enough exercise. I feel generally unhealthy. I have no time for exercise. Though I'm in physical education, the only class that fit into my schedule was yoga. This is a great form of meditation, but I'm unhappy with my body shape. I have to eat in the cafeterias, and I try to eat healthy, but I'm always hungry, especially late at night,...
متن کاملEating poorly, no exercise, and feeling bad about it!
I feel I am getting fat, am eating poorly, and not getting enough exercise. I feel generally unhealthy. I have no time for exercise. Though I'm in physical education, the only class that fit into my schedule was yoga. This is a great form of meditation, but I'm unhappy with my body shape. I have to eat in the cafeterias, and I try to eat healthy, but I'm always hungry, especially late at night,...
متن کاملEating poorly, no exercise, and feeling bad about it!
I feel I am getting fat, am eating poorly, and not getting enough exercise. I feel generally unhealthy. I have no time for exercise. Though I'm in physical education, the only class that fit into my schedule was yoga. This is a great form of meditation, but I'm unhappy with my body shape. I have to eat in the cafeterias, and I try to eat healthy, but I'm always hungry, especially late at night,...
متن کاملEating poorly, no exercise, and feeling bad about it!
I feel I am getting fat, am eating poorly, and not getting enough exercise. I feel generally unhealthy. I have no time for exercise. Though I'm in physical education, the only class that fit into my schedule was yoga. This is a great form of meditation, but I'm unhappy with my body shape. I have to eat in the cafeterias, and I try to eat healthy, but I'm always hungry, especially late at night,...
متن کامل