Running head : PITCH HEIGHT AND TONAL PRIMING 1 Tonal priming

نویسندگان

  • Jon B. Prince
  • Dominique T. Vuvan
  • Mark A. Schmuckler
  • Thomas T. Scott-Clark
  • Jon Prince
چکیده

Research on tonal priming has consistently shown that tonally expected events are processed more efficiently, and confirmed that the locus of the effect is cognitive rather than sensory. However it is also important to investigate the role of pitch height, because models of tonal priming collapse across octave, yet it is possible that pitch height may modulate the effectiveness of tonal priming. We systematically tested this issue by varying the pitch height of a related (tonic) or less-related (subdominant) target chord following a tonal context. Musically untrained participants (N=30) made speeded consonant/dissonant judgements of the final chord of an eight-chord sequence. The effects of tonal priming emerged in accuracy and reaction time measures for all octaves, except for a ceiling effect on accuracy in the matching (Original pitch height) condition. A second experiment increased the shift to two octaves and compressed the chords to eliminate overlap between the target and context chords; tonal priming again emerged. These findings have implications for behavioural study of tonal priming, and support the assumption of octave equivalence in computational models. Running head: PITCH HEIGHT AND TONAL PRIMING 3 Introduction Cognitive processes are fundamentally based on prediction. By predicting the most likely subsequent event, an organism has a better chance at reacting to it in a beneficial way. Some of these expectancies emerge from relatively simple surface information, but predictions are also formed for events derived from deeper structural patterns. For example, an increase in the amplitude of successive sound events creates an expectation for the next sound to be yet louder. This prediction arises from information immediately available at the surface level of the stimulus. Conversely, a semantic word prime need not bear any acoustic similarity to its target (e.g., doctor-nurse). Thus both sensory and cognitive priming contribute to the formation of expectancies. The roles of sensory and cognitive priming are especially important in music, in particular with regard to tonal priming (for a review, see Collins, Tillmann, Barrett, Delbé, & Janata, 2014). This phenomenon typically refers to the fact that for listeners enculturated in Western music, a prime chord (multiple simultaneous notes) creates a strong expectation for a musically related chord (Bharucha & Stoeckig, 1986; Bigand, Poulin, Tillmann, Madurell, & D'Adamo, 2003). This concept is equally applicable to monophonic sequences of single notes, or melodies (Boltz, 1989; Marmel & Tillmann, 2009; Marmel, Tillmann, & Delbe, 2010). In any given musical key, the tonic chord is the most musically expected, psychologically stable, and frequently occurring chord (Krumhansl, 1990). It consists of the most expected/stable/frequent pitch classes in that key. By virtue of this privileged status, listeners are faster and better at processing events that occur synchronously with a tonic chord, regardless of their level of formal musical training (Bigand & Poulin-Charronnat, 2006). These need not be musical tasks – tonal priming is evident in unrelated tasks such as phoneme identification (Bigand, Tillmann, Poulin, D'Adamo, & Madurell, 2001) and even Running head: PITCH HEIGHT AND TONAL PRIMING 4 visual object recognition (Escoffier & Tillmann, 2008). Such findings suggest that tonal priming is modulating central attentional mechanisms rather than music-specific ones. This effect consists of both facilitation of related chords and inhibition of less-related chords (Tillmann, Janata, Birk, & Bharucha, 2003, 2008), rather than a simple graded inhibition whereby the tonic chord interferes the least. Before accepting that tonal priming is a cognitive phenomenon, explanations based on the raw sensory information must be ruled out. The hierarchy of chords in musical stability is correlated with their acoustic similarity, so perhaps sensory-based repetition priming drives the effect rather than a cognitive musical relation. However, tonal priming persists after removing the overlapping frequency spectra of musically related chords, undermining a sensory explanation (Bharucha & Stoeckig, 1987). Additionally, when sensory (repetition priming) and cognitive (tonal priming) are placed in direct conflict, the cognitive prime prevails (Bigand et al., 2003). These authors defined sensory priming as the number of pitch classes shared by the target and preceding musical context (i.e., how often each pitch class occurs). Even when the context shared more tones with the less musically related subdominant chord, the tonic chord was primed the most. Lastly, violating the good continuation of voice-leading (the principle of favouring small pitch changes between subsequent notes and filling in larger leaps, see Meyer, 1973; Narmour, 1990) slows reaction times to targets, but does not influence the effect of tonal priming (Poulin-Charronnat, Bigand, & Madurell, 2005). Nevertheless, sensory priming is still relevant to musical processing – chord repetition can actually decrease the effectiveness of tonal priming (Bigand, Tillmann, PoulinCharronnat, & Manderlier, 2005). Furthermore, at very short delays of 50 ms (half the minimum delay between musical notes, see Huron, 2001), acoustic similarity overpowers musical priming, but by 500 ms the cognitive component is the primary determinant of Running head: PITCH HEIGHT AND TONAL PRIMING 5 priming (Tekman & Bharucha, 1998). Moreover, Leman (2000) proposed a model to account for tonality using short-term memory and periodicity pitch, implying that tonality is in fact a sensory phenomenon and need not involve any cognitive processing. However more recent evidence argues strongly against a purely sensory account of tonal priming (Bigand et al., 2003; Marmel et al., 2010; Vuvan & Schmuckler, 2011). A recent comprehensive review, meta-analysis, and updated modelling of numerous priming studies demonstrates that there is a role of both sensory and cognitive factors in tonal priming, and moreover, that cognitive factors prevail as more influential. (Collins et al., 2014). These authors predicted the strength of priming effects with three factors – periodicity pitch (sensory), the distribution of pitch classes (chroma vector), and the relative activation of musical keys (tonal space). The second and third factors are both cognitive in that they represent derived information not immediately available from the stimulus surface. The cognitive factor of tonal space was the most influential, and the addition of a hybrid hypothesized/empirical probability of closure improved further their modelling of priming effects. Other recent simulation work highlights the role of auditory short-term memory in tonal priming as a way of distinguishing musical and linguistic syntax processing (Bigand, Delbé, Poulin-Charronnat, Leman, & Tillmann, 2014). Despite the attention to the potential contributions of sensory information to music perception, models of tonal priming neglect the role of pitch height. The predominant model, MUSACT (Bharucha, 1987) assumes 12 pitch nodes that correspond to the number of unique pitch classes used in Western music (per octave). Because this model collapses across octave, it cannot address the role of pitch height variation in tonal priming. Without a component of pitch height in MUSACT, a primed chord should receive the same benefit regardless of the octave in which it occurred. Although this model is successful at modelling perceptual data (cf. Collins et al., 2014), including a self-organising neural network instantiation (Tillmann, Running head: PITCH HEIGHT AND TONAL PRIMING 6 Bharucha, & Bigand, 2000), this prediction has not been tested empirically. Because the model proposed by Collins et al. (2014) uses an auditory input as a signal, it should be capable of addressing the role of pitch height. However, despite data based on the model fitting a majority of prior studies, no specific testing of the effect of pitch height on tonal priming has yet been conducted. On the one hand, there are reasons not to expect that changes in pitch height would vitiate tonal priming. Octave equivalence is the concept that pitches with the same class (e.g., C) but played in different octaves are musically equivalent, and is essentially universal across musical cultures (Brown & Jordania, 2013). Experimental dissociations between pitch class and pitch height (Kallman & Massaro, 1979; Shepard, 1982) support octave equivalence, which emerges in 3-month-old infants (Demany & Armand, 1984) and even rhesus monkeys (Wright, Rivera, Hulse, Shyan, & Neiworth, 2000). Moreover, the pitches of a melodic sequence are encoded according to their tonal function rather than their physical properties. For example, detecting an alteration to a pitch was hardest when it changed to a musically related pitch, even though this was also the largest physical change of frequency (Trainor & Trehub, 1994). As tonal priming depends on encoding the tonal function of musical events, it is possible that changes in pitch height will not influence its effectiveness. On the other hand, pitch height is not a trivial characteristic of music, as evidenced by its indispensable role in stream segregation (Bregman, 1990; Dowling, 1973; van Noorden, 1975) and melodic recognition (Deutsch, 2013; Dowling, 1978). Recognising a familiar melody is extremely difficult if the notes are dispersed randomly across different octaves, while retaining their pitch class (Deutsch, 1972; Dowling, 1984; Dowling & Hollombe, 1977). More generally, large changes in pitch height between subsequent melodic notes increases the difficulty of apprehending the tonal structure of a melody (Bharucha, 1984; Cuddy, Cohen, & Mewhort, 1981), which typically use small steps between notes. Whether Running head: PITCH HEIGHT AND TONAL PRIMING 7 pitch height also affects tonal priming is an open question – pitch height can influence the functionality of the tonal hierarchy, which operates well only within the octaves typically used in Western music (Russo, Cuddy, Galembo, & Thompson, 2007). These authors used the standard probe-tone procedure of playing a musical sequence that establishes a musical key, followed by a probe tone at 1 of 12 pitch classes, which listeners rate for goodness-of-fit with the preceding context (Krumhansl & Kessler, 1982). Russo et al. (2007) extended this procedure by testing pitches between E0 and C8 (20-4186 Hz), and found that the standard tonal hierarchy profile is most obvious within the range of C2 to C5 (65-523 Hz). Beyond this range it is increasingly difficult to recover the tonal hierarchy, to the point where it breaks down altogether at the most extreme ranges (the highest and lowest octaves on the piano). The goal of the present research was to assess the validity of the untested assumption of octave equivalence in tonal priming. In other words, does tonal priming still work when the target chord occurs in a different octave? We used a standard paradigm in which a sequence of six chords establishes a musical key (not including the tonic chord), and ends with either the most expected tonic chord, or the less expected (yet consonant) subdominant chord. Participants provided speeded judgements of the final (target) chord, which should be facilitated for tonic chords over subdominant chords. The critical manipulation we added was to vary the pitch height of the target chord. There were three potential outcomes – the pitch height manipulation could have no effect (tonic better than subdominant regardless of octave), or weaken tonal priming (tonic still better but less so at different octaves), or abolish tonal priming altogether (no advantage of tonic over subdominant in other octaves). Experiment 1 Method Participants. There were 30 participants in Experiment 1, recruited from the undergraduate community at Murdoch University, and run individually. Participants were Running head: PITCH HEIGHT AND TONAL PRIMING 8 reimbursed with course credit. The average age was 26.7 (SD = 10.8), and had an average of 3.3 years of formal musical training (SD = 4.0). Apparatus. Custom MATLAB scripts (Mathworks, 2004) using the Psychophysics toolbox (Brainard, 1997) were used to create the experimental interface on a PC running Windows 7. Stimuli were composed in Finale SongWriter 2010 using an acoustic piano timbre and exported to .wav files. Participants were seated in a quiet room and listened to the stimuli over Sennheiser HD280Pro headphones. Stimuli. Twelve unique sequences of 6 chords (the chord context) were prepared that clearly established the musical key of C major (see Figure 1 for an example). The average Maximum Key Correlation (Krumhansl, 1990; Krumhansl & Schmuckler, 1986) was .82 (SD = .05). The duration of each chord was 500 ms. The tonic chord (C major) did not occur in the chord context; any other diatonic triad was allowed. This arrangement is equivalent to the “subdominant in context” condition of Bigand et al. (2003), in which the less-related subdominant triad could occur in the context, but not the tonic. Two additional chords followed each chord context, comprised of either a dominant and tonic chord (V-I cadence) or a tonic and subdominant chord (I-IV cadence). The tonic chord is the most musically expected, whereas the subdominant is less expected (but still legal). In terms of cognitive and sensory priming, the tonic chord receives the strongest cognitive priming, whereas sensory priming is strongest for the subdominant chord, as it was heard earlier in the sequence. Thus harmonic relatedness was manipulated by varying which chord pair occurred after the context. Importantly, both cadence types (V-I and I-IV) are identical transitions when presented outside of a preceding musical context. For example, in the key of C major, a V-I cadence consists of a G major chord followed by a C major chord; however in G major the exact same chords define a I-IV cadence. Thus the manipulation of harmonic relatedness is not confounded with the identity of the final (target) chord. Any Running head: PITCH HEIGHT AND TONAL PRIMING 9 difference between harmonic relatedness conditions can therefore only be a result of the preceding 6-chord context that primes the tonic chord over the subdominant chord (via cognitive priming) or the reverse (via sensory priming). The participants’ task was to judge whether the final chord was consonant or dissonant. Adding an additional note to a chord that does not belong in the key of C major makes it sound dissonant (subjectively rough, unpleasing, or clashing). Consonant chords had no added notes and therefore sounded smooth, pleasing, or not clashing. For half of the dissonant trials, the added note was one semitone above the root of the chord (#1), for the other half it was one semitone above the fifth, or top note of the triad (#5). The added note was always within the pitch range of the consonant target chord, as shown in Figure 1. Each participant heard an equal number of consonant and dissonant trials. The manipulation of primary interest was that of the target’s pitch height. The final chord (target) was presented either in its original pitch register, shifted up one octave, or shifted down one octave (see Figure 1). Crossing the 12 sequences with the 2 levels of harmonic relatedness, 2 levels of consonance, and 3 levels of pitch height resulted in a total of 144 conditions. Participants heard each condition twice, giving 288 trials. There were three versions of each sequence, transposed to either the key of C, F, or G major. Assignment of sequences to keys was counterbalanced such that each participant heard an equal number of sequences in each key. Insert Figure 1 about here Procedure. Participants first provided informed consent and completed a background questionnaire on musical experience. Subsequently, they were seated in the experimental room and received instructions on the task, which was to indicate whether the final chord of a sequence was consonant or dissonant. The experimenter explained the concept of consonance and dissonance, and participants did 24 practice trials in which a single chord was presented, Running head: PITCH HEIGHT AND TONAL PRIMING 10 which was either consonant or dissonant (as defined above). The experimenter was present throughout the practice trials in order to answer questions. Once the participants understood the task and completed the practice trials, they continued to the full experiment (288 trials, mixed design). The experimenter debriefed the participants once they completed the experiment, which took approximately 30 minutes. Results Participants performed well at the task, with an average proportion correct of .85 (SD = .09). Valid reaction times (from correct trials only) averaged 484 ms (SD = 18). Accuracy and reaction time correlated negatively (r = -.32), ruling out a speed-accuracy tradeoff. All subsequent analyses included the consonant trials only, as the added out-of-key notes in dissonant trials defeats the manipulation of harmonic relatedness. A 2 (harmonic relatedness: tonic or subdominant) by 3 (target pitch height: High, Original, or Low) repeated measures ANOVA on the accuracy data revealed significant main effects for both factors and a significant interaction; these effects appear in Figure 2. The main effect of harmonic relatedness was that participants were more accurate for tonic than subdominant trials, F(1,29) = 28.1, p = .003, η = .06. The main effect of pitch height emerged from better performance on the Original pitch height condition than the High and Low conditions, F(2,58) = 15.9, p < .001, η = .26 (High and Low conditions differed by only .026, pairwise comparison p = 1). The interaction, F(2,58) = 6.3, p < .001, η = .03, revealed that there was no effect of harmonic relatedness on accuracy in the Original pitch height condition, t(29) = .53, p = .6, whereas there was in the High condition, t(29) = 2.93, p = .007, and the Low condition, t(29) = 4.62, p < .001. Insert Figure 2 about here The same ANOVA analysis on the reaction time data also showed main effects of harmonic relatedness, F(1,29) = 39.6, p < .001, η = .19, and pitch height F(2,58) = 3.3, p = Running head: PITCH HEIGHT AND TONAL PRIMING 11 .045, η = .05; these data also appear in Figure 2. As with the accuracy data, participants did better for tonic trials and for the Original pitch height condition, although the only significant pairwise difference was between the Low and Original pitch height condition, M difference = 64 ms, p = .04. There was again an interaction F(2,58) = 3.2, p = .049, η = .02, in this case because the effect of harmonic relatedness was smaller in the High pitch height condition, however tonic and subdominant conditions were significantly different at all pitch heights, t(29) = 2.78, p = .01 (High), t(29) = 5.35, p < .001 (Original), t(29) = 4.57, p < .001 (Low). Expertise analysis. To determine if musical expertise influenced the effectiveness of tonal priming or its immunity to pitch height changes, years of formal training was correlated with difference scores (between tonic and subdominant relatedness conditions) for accuracy and RT. There were no significant correlations, indicating that musical training did not influence the strength of tonal priming in any pitch height condition (highest r = .24, p = .20). Discussion Changing the pitch height of a sequence-final target chord impaired the overall accuracy and reaction time on a speeded consonant/dissonant judgement, similar to the effect of violating voice leading (Poulin-Charronnat et al., 2005). Manipulating the harmonic relatedness also influenced performance, with participants performing better for targets that were more musically expected, regardless of musical experience. Thus cognitive priming of musical expectations overrode effects of sensory (repetition) priming (Bigand et al., 2003) and was not dependent on formal musical training (Bigand & Poulin-Charronnat, 2006). Most critically, the effect of harmonic relatedness emerged at all target pitch height conditions (in accuracy and/or reaction time), demonstrating that tonal priming is not limited to the pitch range of the preceding context. These findings confirm and extend existing tonal priming effects in the literature, which heretofore have assumed octave equivalence. These data are Running head: PITCH HEIGHT AND TONAL PRIMING 12 the first to address this assumption directly, and argue in the affirmative for tonal priming being impervious to pitch height manipulations. The accuracy data of the Original pitch height condition showed no effect of tonal priming , likely due to a ceiling effect, (accuracy was 95% and error bars were smallest in this pitch height condition). However, the reaction time measure showed strong tonal priming. Thus maintaining the same pitch height for the target made the task sufficiently easy that manipulating harmonic relatedness had no observable effect on the accuracy measure, instead affecting reaction time. The effect of tonal priming on reaction time was significant at all pitch height conditions, but nominally smallest in the High pitch height condition, suggesting that tonal priming could diminish further at larger pitch height changes. However, the Low pitch height condition argues for the robustness of tonal priming across pitch height, as it observed no reduction in the strength of tonal priming whatsoever (compared to the Original condition with no pitch height change). Yet here too there may be a problem, because participants noted that targets in the Low pitch height condition sounded “muddy,” or more dissonant than targets in the Original or High pitch height conditions. It is unclear how this phenomenon might affect tonal priming, and moreover, if the absolute pitch range of the context and target is important. These findings therefore warrant further investigation on how pitch height might influence tonal priming. That is, what are the roles of the size of the pitch height change and the absolute pitch range in determining whether tonal priming is functional? Experiment 2 was designed to answer these questions. Experiment 2 Although Experiment 1 strongly suggests that pitch height has a negligible (if any) impact on tonal priming, the findings raised further questions. In Experiment 1 the oneRunning head: PITCH HEIGHT AND TONAL PRIMING 13 octave change did not eliminate the pitch range overlap – for instance, the two lowest notes of the target chord matched the pitch range of the two upper notes of the penultimate chord (for the High condition – vice versa for the Low condition). Thus despite the change of pitch register, the remaining overlap may have fostered tonal priming effects. In Experiment 2 the shift was increased to two octaves in order to test if tonal priming remains when the pitch height change is dramatically larger. For the downward shift condition, the chord context was transposed up two octaves and the target remained in the original (untransposed) position, to eliminate the muddy dissonant sound of the lowest chords in Experiment 1. Observing tonal priming effects in these conditions would provide strong validation of the octave equivalence assumption in tonal priming models. Method Participants. A new set of 30 participants from the same pool were recruited for Experiment 2, with an average age of 26.6 (SD = 9.5), and 3.8 average years of formal musical training (SD = 4.7). Apparatus. All apparatus were the same as Experiment 1. Stimuli. The three pitch height conditions were: target up two octaves, target at the original pitch height, and target down two octaves. For the last condition, the context was shifted up two octaves, and the pitch height of the target remained unchanged (thus two octaves lower than the expected pitch height). The chord sequences were compacted into “block” chords that spanned less than an octave, considerably smaller than the chords of Experiment 1 (see Figure 3). This compression maximises the gap between the context and target, in order to prevent pitch height overlap from influencing the data. In fact, the empty gap between the last two chords in the pitch change conditions (e.g., for the High condition, the distance between the highest Running head: PITCH HEIGHT AND TONAL PRIMING 14 note of the penultimate chord to the lowest note of the target) averaged 17 semitones (SD = 6.0). The design was the same as Experiment 1, with 12 sequences, 2 levels of harmonic relatedness, 2 levels of consonance, and 3 levels of pitch height. Again there were two repetitions of all 144 conditions, resulting in 288 trials overall. Insert Figure 3 about here Procedure. The procedure was identical to Experiment 1. Results The average proportion correct was .88 (SD = .08), and the average reaction time of valid trials was 487 ms (SD = 22). There was no evidence of a speed-accuracy tradeoff, as accuracy and reaction time were negatively correlated, r = -.19. Again only consonant trials were analysed. A 2 (harmonic relatedness) by 3 (pitch height) repeated measures ANOVA of the accuracy data recovered a main effect of relatedness, F(1,29) = 11.4, p = .002, η = .06, because accuracy for the tonic was higher than the subdominant (as in Experiment 1; see Figure 4). There was also a main effect of pitch height, F(2,58) = 19.6, p < .001, η = .29, due to best performance for Original pitch height targets, intermediate for High targets, and worst for Low targets (all pairwise comparisons significant p < .05). There was no interaction between relatedness and pitch height, F(2,58) < 1, ns. Insert Figure 4 about here Repeating the same analysis with the valid reaction time data revealed a main effect of relatedness, F(1,29) = 7.8, p = .009, η = .05 (faster for tonic targets than subdominant). There was also an effect of pitch height, F(2,58) = 3.9, p = .03, η = .07 because participants were faster for High targets than Low, M difference = 53 ms, p = .048 (Medium differed neither from High nor Low, p = .38 and .49, respectively). There was no interaction, F(2,58) = 1.4, p = .25, η = .01. Running head: PITCH HEIGHT AND TONAL PRIMING 15 Expertise analysis. The analysis correlating accuracy and RT difference scores between tonic and subdominant relatedness conditions with years of formal training (as in Experiment 1) showed no significant relationships between the variables (highest r = .28, p = .14). Therefore there was no connection between the strength of tonal priming and musical expertise. Discussion In Experiment 1, the wide chord voicings and smaller octave shift meant that there was always some overlap between the penultimate chord and the target, which may have aided the perseveration of tonal priming. Compressing the chords and doubling the octave shift in Experiment 2 eliminated any remaining overlap in the High and Low conditions, yet tonal priming was still operational, regardless of the target’s pitch height. Furthermore, and unlike Experiment 1, the effects of tonal priming did not differ across pitch height conditions. In other words, tonal priming persists despite extreme changes in pitch height. Again there were no effects of musical expertise, reinforcing the ubiquitous nature of tonal priming across Western listeners regardless of training. Together, these findings strongly endorse tonal priming as being immune to large variations in pitch height. The main effect of relatedness on RT was smaller in Experiment 2. However, the critical question is if the effect of relatedness changes across pitch height conditions, not across experiment. Accordingly, the lack of an interaction between these factors (in both experiments) is the most important evidence of the resilience of tonal priming across pitch height. Given that changing the pitch height of the target decreased participants’ accuracy, it is reasonable to predict that larger changes would result in greater performance penalties. It is therefore surprising that the advantage of the Original condition was actually smaller in Experiment 2 – the lines representing the Low and High condition in Figure 4 are closer to Running head: PITCH HEIGHT AND TONAL PRIMING 16 the Original condition than they are in Figure 2 (Experiment 1). To be clear, shifting the pitch height of the target made consonance/dissonance judgements more difficult, but it caused no attenuation in tonal priming. Indeed, the harmonic relatedness effect is nominally (but not significantly) strongest in the target-up condition, entirely inconsistent with the notion that tonal priming would decrease when the context and target are at different pitch heights. On the whole, these data provide no reason to expect that further increases in pitch height would be detrimental to tonal priming. Moreover, such large shifts are virtually nonexistent in standard Western music, and anything beyond two octaves would be musically untenable (indeed, even these shifts are not particularly musical). General Discussion Two experiments were conducted to investigate how pitch height differences between context and target chords might affect tonal priming. Experiment 1 used a conventional paradigm to test if shifting the target chord up or down an octave influenced the effectiveness of tonal priming. Although pitch height changes impaired performance overall, the effects of tonal priming remained in both accuracy and reaction time measures. Experiment 2 used a larger shift (+/-2 octaves) of the target to eliminate pitch register overlap with the context; tonal priming still occurred in these cases. These data strongly endorse the consistency of tonal priming in the face of pitch height manipulations. The primary motivation of this research was to test if the assumption of octave equivalence in the tonal priming literature was valid. This issue is particularly relevant to computational models of tonal expectancies that assume octave equivalence, in particular MUSACT (Bharucha, 1987). Indeed, in its current form this model cannot predict any effect of pitch height whatsoever, although given the present findings it would be possible to extend MUSACT by adding an independent modular component of pitch height. Of course, the more recent model of Collins et al. (2014) automatically includes pitch height via the periodicity Running head: PITCH HEIGHT AND TONAL PRIMING 17 pitch component. Despite the near-universality and early developmental trajectory of octave equivalence, however, there has been some controversy regarding the existence of octave equivalence in psychophysical research (for a discussion, see Burns, 1999), and some evidence that musical training develops the phenomenon (Krumhansl & Shepard, 1979). Nevertheless, more recent research shows that failures to observe octave equivalence are most likely due to methodological difficulties, and that the phenomenon is not dependent on training (Hoeschele, Weisman, & Sturdy, 2012). The present findings concur with this perspective – although moving the target to a different octave decreased performance overall, the tonal function was equivalent regardless. Although this research discounts the effect of pitch height on tonal priming, it is unquestionable that pitch height is a fundamental component of music perception, affecting the very identity of a musical excerpt, indicating emotional cues (Huron, Kinney, & Precoda, 2006; Juslin & Laukka, 2003), and influencing stream segregation (Bregman, 1990; Dowling, 1973). Indeed, the sequence of changes in pitch height, otherwise known as melodic contour, is a primary component in the perception of musical pitch (Deutsch, 2013; Dowling, 1978; Schmuckler, 2004, 2009). Additionally, the most comprehensive analysis of the tonal priming literature argues for a role (albeit subsidiary) of the sensory characteristic of periodicity pitch (Collins et al., 2014). The independence of pitch height and tonal priming was not a foregone conclusion. Initial research in tonality showed that pitch height can influence the relative stability of different pitch classes (Krumhansl, 1979; Krumhansl & Shepard, 1979). Specifically, for some listeners, probe pitches closer in pitch height to the preceding context received higher goodness of fit ratings. Subsequent work used circular tones (Shepard tones) that obscure any notion of pitch height (Shepard, 1964), enabling studying tonality independently of pitch height effects (Krumhansl & Kessler, 1982). Other behavioural research showed that Running head: PITCH HEIGHT AND TONAL PRIMING 18 introducing more complex contours reduces the ability to extract the underlying harmonic structure of a melody (Cuddy et al., 1981), and that anchoring out-of-key tones to in-key tones depended critically on their pitch height proximity (Bharucha, 1984). Additionally, the variable strength of the tonal hierarchy in octaves outside the range of typical music shows that tonality is not impervious to changes in pitch height (Russo et al., 2007). The vast majority (97%) of the pitches tested in these experiments were within the octave range in which Russo et al. successfully recovered the tonal hierarchy (octaves 1-6), so it is perhaps expected that tonality (and thus tonal priming) was fully operational. It is nonetheless surprising that there was no decrease in tonal priming at the highest octaves, as would be predicted by a decreased role of tonality as pitches strayed beyond the typical bounds of music (octaves 2-5). More generally, introducing drastic changes to pitch height is not simply a sensory change – it violates the voice-leading principles of music. Again, previous research showed that violating voice-leading makes processing musical chords more challenging, but has no effect on tonal priming (Poulin-Charronnat et al., 2005). In the present studies our manipulation of pitch height necessarily also changed voice-leading, and thus our findings show that tonal priming is immune to concomitant changes to voice-leading and pitch height. Like voice-leading, preserving normal pitch height is also a learned schematic expectation, and the present data showed an overall cost to changing the pitch height. But the other schematic expectation of tonal relatedness remained intact regardless of the change in pitch height, dissociating these two types of schematic expectation. In other words, pitch height and tonal relatedness are not general features that contribute to an overall sense of schematic expectation, but are demonstrably independent processes in the perception of music. This perspective is reinforced by the successful regression modelling of tonal priming using independent predictors (Collins et al., 2014). Running head: PITCH HEIGHT AND TONAL PRIMING 19 Neuroimaging research substantiates the independence of pitch height and pitch class, as changing them activated distinct areas of auditory cortex (Warren, Uppenkamp, Patterson, & Griffiths, 2003). Furthermore, although areas of rostromedial frontal cortex show specific activation patterns to different musical keys, the exact mapping is flexible –the topography of which key aligned to which activation pattern was not the same in different scanning sessions (Janata et al., 2002). Thus brain activity in response to manipulations of tonality seems unaffected by the exact pitch height. With specific regard to tonal priming (using the same speeded consonant/dissonant judgement task), the inferior frontal cortex responds differently for related and unrelated chords (Tillmann, Janata, & Bharucha, 2003). Each chord used in these experiments consisted of several pitches that extended over an interval of at least 7 semitones (cf Figure 3), and sometimes nearly 3 octaves (cf Figure 1). It is uncertain if tonal priming remains resistant to pitch height changes in a context with a much smaller pitch range, such as a monophonic melody, which consists of single notes. Tonal priming is functional in a melodic context (Marmel, Perrin, & Tillmann, 2011; Marmel & Tillmann, 2009; Marmel et al., 2010; Marmel, Tillmann, & Dowling, 2008), but melodies rarely exceed a pitch range of 1.5 octaves (typically much smaller). Future research should focus on testing the effect of pitch height on tonal priming in a melodic context. These data argue against a role of pitch height, but assumptions are always worth testing, and replications particularly valuable in the current scientific context (Pashler & Wagenmakers, 2012). The primary message from this research is that tonal priming is resistant to changes in pitch height (within the tested context). This is a reassuring finding and represents an important contribution to the literature on tonality in music cognition. It supports the notion that tonal priming is primarily a cognitive rather than a sensory one (Bigand et al., 2003). But Running head: PITCH HEIGHT AND TONAL PRIMING 20 these experiments also raise questions about the integration of schematic expectations and prompt further research in this area to support the tonal priming literature. Running head: PITCH HEIGHT AND TONAL PRIMING 21

برای دانلود متن کامل این مقاله و بیش از 32 میلیون مقاله دیگر ابتدا ثبت نام کنید

ثبت نام

اگر عضو سایت هستید لطفا وارد حساب کاربری خود شوید

منابع مشابه

Tone sandhi and tonal coarticulation in Fuzhou Min

This study examines the completeness of Fuzhou tonal neutralisation and its interaction with tonal coarticulation. In Fuzhou, Tone 44, 242 and 53 are allegedly neutralised into Tone 44 preceding Tone 53. Acoustic data, analysed with Linear Mixed Modelling, show no significant difference in pitch height between neutralised tones, suggesting complete neutralisation in production. A forcedchoice i...

متن کامل

Estimating the time to reach a target frequency in singing.

The ability to match pitches quickly and accurately is essential for proficient singing. We describe a new technique for estimating the time to reach a target frequency that uses adaptive optimal-kernel (AOK) time-frequency representations, designed to optimize the time-frequency tradeoff at each time point. We show in two experiments that this measure is more sensitive to tonal priming effects...

متن کامل

Tonal targets in early child English, Spanish, and Catalan.

This study analyses the scaling and alignment of low and high intonational targets in the speech of 27 children - nine English-speaking, nine Catalan-speaking and nine Spanish-speaking - between the ages of two and six years. We compared the intonational patterns of words controlled for number of syllables and stress position in the child speech to the adult target speech provided by their moth...

متن کامل

Running head: TASK AND PITCH STRUCTURE The effect of task and pitch structure on pitch-time interactions in music

Musical pitch-time relations were explored by investigating the effect of temporal variation on musical pitch processing. In Experiment 1, musicians heard a standard tone, followed by a tonal context, then followed by a comparison tone, and performed one of two tasks. In the cognitive task, listeners were asked to ignore the standard tone and to judge whether the comparison tone was in the key ...

متن کامل

Focus in production: tonal shape, intensity and word order.

The effect of word order and prosodic focus on the tonal shape and intensity in the production of prosody was studied. The results show that the production of focus in Finnish follows a global pattern with regard to tonal features. The relative pitch height difference between contrasted words is the most important pitch-related factor in signaling narrow prosodic focus. Narrow focus is not loca...

متن کامل

ذخیره در منابع من


  با ذخیره ی این منبع در منابع من، دسترسی به آن را برای استفاده های بعدی آسان تر کنید

برای دانلود متن کامل این مقاله و بیش از 32 میلیون مقاله دیگر ابتدا ثبت نام کنید

ثبت نام

اگر عضو سایت هستید لطفا وارد حساب کاربری خود شوید

عنوان ژورنال:

دوره   شماره 

صفحات  -

تاریخ انتشار 2015